Liz R. Kover
Professor Amy Cook
LIF 580
August 15th, 2014
A
Whisper To A Scream:
Contradictions
and Curiosities in the War Against Cesar Millan
“All the scientist creates in a fact is the
language in which he enunciates it.”
– Henri Poincare
Any positive reinforcement trainer
will tell you that ‘you kill more bees with honey than vinegar’. In other
words, offer positive feedback and watch desirable behavior increase in
frequency; sling mud and you’ll see what is undesirable
rise up. I wonder then, why the behavioral experts who so vehemently disapprove
of Cesar Millan would attempt to “train him out of ” his ways using punishment
as their go-to method. If saving dogs from (perceived) harm is truly the
mission of the movement against “dog whispering”, why not employ methods that
are strictly positive in dealing with the perceived problem? Could eliciting a
response of ‘learned helplessness’ be the real goal? Attempt to break the
spirit of a successful “newcomer” by claiming he is reviving disproven,
antiquated ways from the dark recesses of history when in fact, he is bringing
to light something that before him had yet to be illuminated? There are
millions upon millions of people who proclaim that life for them and their dogs
has changed for the better in light of Cesar Millan’s teachings and techniques.
It seems very insulting to assume that all of these people simply “don’t know
better”, or have blindly bought into the devious scam of an illusionist. I see
something altogether different, and that is an historical pattern of “schools
of thought” vying for the position of Pack Leader within academic circles.
While Cesar Millan
is the present focal point of controversy, the debate between behaviorism - and
ideologies that threaten to reveal its limitations - is long-standing. Modern
day positive reinforcement dog training evolved from Skinnerian roots, and has
changed very little at the core of its ideology. Skinner maintained that
“behavior is shaped and maintained by its consequences”, and that as the
consequences contingent on behavior are investigated, more and more “they are
taking over the explanatory functions previously assigned to personalities,
states of mind, feelings, traits of character, purposes, and intentions”
(Chomsky, 1971). Skinner’s confidence that scientific inquiry into behavior
would continue to strip away the existence of autonomous intention seemed unshakable.
Until he attempted to cram the dynamic innateness of language acquisition into
a Skinner box, at which point the foundation of his logic began to crack.
Actually, the logic itself isn’t flawed; the fallacy is in professing that
logic alone explains the “internal state” that Cesar Millan refers to as,
simply, energy. “Dogs use energy to communicate. Energy is what I call beingness.
It is who and what we are in every moment” (Cesar’s Way, web).
Inarguably,
B.F. Skinner sought to eradicate words like “beingness” and “energy” from the
vernacular he so carefully ascribed to behavior. These words implied the
existence of emotional states in both human and animal, which would not fit
inside the confines of laboratory conditions. A ‘technology of behavior is
available’, Skinner argued, which would more successfully reduce the aversive
consequences of behavior…and maximize the achievements of which the (human)
organism is capable. But the “defenders of freedom oppose its use” (Skinner, p.
125). Undoubtedly Skinner would’ve seen Cesar Millan as a defender of freedom –
as do I, which is why his principles ring so true.
When
the intellectual quandary of language acquisition arose, Skinner grasped
desperately to keep the abstractions of “beingness” under a tightly closed lid.
But alas, Nature is messy and spills over. Try as science might to contain it,
the vast scope of “freedom and dignity” that arises innately from within the
experience of living, behaving, and “being” will always be one step ahead of
our ability to objectively explain it. And, just as the science of cognitive
psychology evolved to bring internal states of mentalism into the realm of
scientific validity, the emerging, interdisciplinary science of Anthrozoology
aims to understand both human and animal behavior in context of how “human
beingness” relates to, affects, and is in part defined by, the beingness of
other animal species. The human-canine relationship is at the heart of this
developing field. According to the Animals & Society Institute, Human-Animal
Studies (HAS) is a rapidly growing interdisciplinary field that examines the
complex and multidimensional relationships between humans and other animals.
HAS comprises work in several disciplines in the social sciences (sociology,
anthropology, psychology, political science) the humanities (history, literary
criticism, philosophy, geography), and the natural sciences (ethology,
veterinary medicine, animal welfare science, and comparative psychology) (http://www.animalsandsociety.org/pages/human-animal-studies).
Being that the modern science of dog behavior is so focused on the
co-evolutionary influence our respective species have upon one another, I
imagine that what critics today claim are wrong or “anecdotal observations” of Cesar
Millan’s about the influence of a person’s energy on the behavior of his dog,
are actually the scientific data of tomorrow.
Any whose
contributions to a well-established field challenge its hard-won tenure in the
“lay consciousness” faces severe opposition. One such revolutionary is Dr. Jane
Goodall. In 1960, Jane was in her early twenties when the man known as the
father of anthropology, Louis Leakey, selected her and two other young women
with no formal education – Dian Fossey and Beruti Galdikas - to venture to
Africa, observe apes in the wild, and report back on what they saw. Leakey chose these three specifically
for the absence of academia’s influence, which kept them unbiased, and unlikely
to have expectations related to a predetermined hypothesis.
The successful
objectification of behavioral observation was not merely a breakthrough in a field,
it marked the foundation for a field of its kind to exist in the first place.
It is not surprising then that when Jane Goodall - a young woman who had never
heard the word ethology when she unwittingly helped to reshape it - was
inserted into the Ph.D. program in ethology at Cambridge University in the
early sixties, she was ridiculed and cast out by the academic elite. Said Jane,
“It was a bit shocking to be told I had done everything wrong. Everything.” (David Quammen, National
Geographic Magazine, 2010). She
writes in National Geographic Magazine, “I believed that having a degree of
empathy for my subjects could help me detect slight changes in their mood or
attitudes and provide insights into their complex social processes. I think
time has proved me right.”
Critics accused Jane
Goodall of scientific sacrilege for her heart-centered approach, by which she
ascribed personalities to wild chimpanzees, and wrote about the exchanges of
energy between them and her as they accepted her into their community across
species boundaries. But it was exactly who she was and how she did her work
that led to her allowance into the chimpanzee colony by its patriarch, whom she
called David Greybeard. And it was because of her acceptance into their world
that “the definition of man” is what it is today. Jane Goodall may not have had
the “seal of approval” from the academic elite back then, but in trusting
instinct to inform intellect, she understood more than could be expressed in
the confining language of science.
Dr.
Goodall’s work was instrumental in shifting paradigms away from the mechanics
of animal behavior to the far “messier” internal dimensions of animal life and
relationships. As Cesar Millan explains dogs’ behavior in the context of their
unique canine-human-family-packs, Dr. Goodall exposed chimpanzee behavior as
seen in the interrelationships of one wild chimpanzee colony. Similarly, Cesar Millan
explains dogs’ behavior as being inextricably interrelated with their “packs” -
or modern day social groups comprised of human family members and other
animals.
Behaviorism
states that it is an animal’s environment that predicts its behavior, but
“positive reinforcement training” does not take into account the role of other
dogs and “family members” on the behavior of each individual in the group.
So much of the
anti-Cesar Millan rhetoric boils down to semantics. For example, people jump
all over his use of the word “alpha”. Simply stated, the alpha male or female
is the member of a group of social animals that holds the highest rank. This is
what CM means when he talks about a pack, and about holding the leadership role
within your pack. If human beings aren’t the “alpha” members in a family pack
that involves both people and pets, everyone is in trouble. The animal in this
position within the social system may have preferential access to mates, food
or space. The term was popularized with regard to the gray wolf by L. David
Mech in 1970, (and later disavowed by Mech himself, who said his observations
of captive wolf groups comprised of unrelated members wouldn’t accurately
reflect the behavior of wild wolves or domesticated dogs). What he discovered
instead was that the alpha members of a wolf pack are a breeding pair, and they
and their offspring form a pack. Mech said of gray wolves, “In our experience,
the most usual context of dominance behavior in free-ranging wolves is that of
parent to offspring” (Mech, David and Cluff, Dean, Prolonged Intensive
Dominance Behavior Between Gray Wolves, 2010). That being said, Cesar Millan’s
metaphor of dog+human family members = pack makes perfect sense.
A
study from 2008 out of the U. of Florida suggests that wolves outperform dogs
in following human social cues under certain conditions. “Although earlier
studies suggested that wolves could not follow even simple cues such as tapping
or proximal pointing, later research indicated that highly socialized
hand-reared wolves could find food under easier conditions, such as when a
container indicating the correct location was touched by a human (Udell, Dorey
and Clive, Wolves outperform dogs in following human social cues, Animal
Behaviour, 2008). While this does not prove that wolves and dogs are “the same”,
it does suggest a correlation between the effects of environment and rearing
conditions on canines – wolves and dogs alike – which might imply that the
social structure comprised of a human-canine family, or “interdependent interspecies
pack” must be analyzed by a different set of guidelines altogether than wild
wolves, captive wolves, wild domesticated dogs or domesticated dogs as
independent groups. Because humans and dogs have co-evolved since the “caveman
hunted alongside the wolf”, our respective evolutionary paths have been
inextricably linked and undeniably one has influenced the other in fundamental
ways. It only makes sense that CM’s metaphor of people-and-dog-family-pack will
be the hybrid human-canine social structure that Anthrozoologists will soon
define in scientific terms.
Contrary to the
misinformed claims that CM is a domineering authoritarian overseer of his pack,
he teaches us about calm-assertive leadership. Any regular viewer of Dog
Whisperer knows that e-collars, alpha rolls and dominance plays in response to
aggressive dogs in attack mode comprise a very small percentage of CM’s
dealings with dogs. According to the infamous bell curve, there will always be
extreme cases on either end of a spectrum, and while 100% positive methods will
work in 96% of cases, those fringe 4% will require the use of alternative
methods. In response to misrepresentations about his “signature methods”, CM
and the producers of Dog Whisperer did a show-by-show breakdown, watching
hundreds of hours of television and counting when a particular technique was
used in any given episode. They examined in detail 317 separate cases of
problem dog behavior across 140 episodes, and charted which of CM’s dog
rehabilitation methods and tools were used across the board.
Critics may be
surprised to learn that CM never once introduced a prong collar, introduced a
choke chain in only 1% of cases and an e-collar in 3% of cases. In 17% of
cases, the owners chose to use a prong collar. Cesar teaches that any and all
of these items are tools which, if used correctly, can be effective. But that
it is the energy with which we communicate calm-assertive leadership to our
dogs that makes the real difference. Calm-assertive leadership was discussed in
98% of episodes, discussion of body language in 91% of episodes, the importance
of exercise in 72% of episodes, the power of using other dogs to help balance
the troubled dog’s energy – or what CM calls the “power of the pack” was covered
in 63% of episodes, and positive
reinforcement was used in 67% of episodes!
Those who
misunderstand CM claim that CM looks at dogs as though they are “in an
ego-driven race to become the leaders of the free-world”, and that “it is only
when we misinterpret canine behavior that we start to think dogs must be trying
to achieve a higher rank than us.” (Stilwell, Train Your Dog Positively, p.21).
CM teaches that dogs must respect humans as “pack leaders” in order to maintain
structure in a household. Whether one is a mother or father, the President of
the United States, a dog trainer, or a dog’s human guardians, leadership
qualities are obviously qualities one should procure and project to those who
depend on them for guidance, protection and direction. “To positively influence
your dog’s behavior, you must always begin by being a positive, confident, calm
and assertive human. This is the definition of true leadership” (Cesar’s Rules).
Let us look
at the terminology CM uses for which he is so harshly criticized in terms of
parenting. Three themes can be identified in assessments of parenting style
over the past 50 years (Skinner, Johnson & Snyder, 2005). The first tenet
of a motivational relationship between parent and child is love and affection.
Many who denounce CM and call him cruel are very wrong in their estimation that
he withholds affection. He simply instructs people to give affection at the
appropriate times, so as to reinforce a calm state of mind. This is akin in
positive reinforcement to ignoring behavior that is undesirable while giving
attention to that which is desirable.
The second theme
is parent provision of structure. Referred to in work on discipline and
authoritarian parenting, this theme suggests that clear and consistent
expectations and limit setting are advantageous to children, especially in
terms of their internalization of rules and the development of self-efficacy
(Skinner, Johnson and Snyder, Six Dimensions of Parenting: A Motivational
Model, Parenting: Science and Practice, V. 5, No. 2, April-June, 2005). CM says
the role of the pack leader is to set “rules, boundaries and limitations”. Dogs
need to know that their pack leader is clearly setting the rules, boundaries
and limitations for their life both inside and outside the house. -
Anger, aggression, or abuse toward the dog will not establish you as pack
leader; an angry, aggressive leader is not in control. Calm-assertive energy
and daily, consistent leadership behavior will make enforcing the rules easier
(Cesar Millan, Be The Pack Leader).
In preeminent positive reinforcement trainer Dr. Sophia Yin’s “Program for developing leadership in humans and impulse control in dogs”, Dr. Yin says “you gain leadership by controlling all the resources that motivate the pet and requiring the pet willingly work for play, treats and pets instead of getting them for free” (Yin, Teaching Fido to Learn, loc 44 of 370/kindle book). Here, Dr. Yin uses terminology that alludes to an alpha wolf or other animal playing a dominant role in a group who controls the resources, and therefore creates order among pack members. Patricia McConnell says “the key is to understand that dogs will work to get something they want…make obedience relevant to life, so that your dog begins to learn: “Oh I see, the way to control my environment and get what I want is to do what she asks.” Cesar says essentially the same thing: “Waiting is another way that pack leaders assert their position. Puppies wait to eat, and adult dogs wait until the pack leader wants them to travel. Waiting is a form of psychological work for the dog. Domestication means dogs don’t need to hunt for food, but they can still work for it. Establish your position as pack leader by asking your dog to work” (Cesar Millan, Be The Pack Leader). CM talks about the critical importance of being the pack leader precisely so that your dog will not feel responsible for creating order out of chaos, and will understand what is expected of him, so that he can work to earn the resources you control.
In preeminent positive reinforcement trainer Dr. Sophia Yin’s “Program for developing leadership in humans and impulse control in dogs”, Dr. Yin says “you gain leadership by controlling all the resources that motivate the pet and requiring the pet willingly work for play, treats and pets instead of getting them for free” (Yin, Teaching Fido to Learn, loc 44 of 370/kindle book). Here, Dr. Yin uses terminology that alludes to an alpha wolf or other animal playing a dominant role in a group who controls the resources, and therefore creates order among pack members. Patricia McConnell says “the key is to understand that dogs will work to get something they want…make obedience relevant to life, so that your dog begins to learn: “Oh I see, the way to control my environment and get what I want is to do what she asks.” Cesar says essentially the same thing: “Waiting is another way that pack leaders assert their position. Puppies wait to eat, and adult dogs wait until the pack leader wants them to travel. Waiting is a form of psychological work for the dog. Domestication means dogs don’t need to hunt for food, but they can still work for it. Establish your position as pack leader by asking your dog to work” (Cesar Millan, Be The Pack Leader). CM talks about the critical importance of being the pack leader precisely so that your dog will not feel responsible for creating order out of chaos, and will understand what is expected of him, so that he can work to earn the resources you control.
The third theme
is that of autonomy support, suggesting that better developmental outcomes
accrue if parents (pack leaders) interact with children (dogs) in ways that do
not compromise their freedom of expression or intrinsic motivation (Skinner,
Johnson and Snyder, Six Dimensions of Parenting: A Motivational Model,
Parenting: Science and Practice, V. 5, No. 2, April-June, 2005). While many dog
trainers focus only on conditioning responses to human cues, CM stresses the
importance of fulfilling your dog’s needs on the “animal” level, as well as the
breed-specific level. Meaning, if you have a herding dog, that dog is
hard-wired for running and corralling animals; it is in his blood. If you do
not live on a farm with a flock of sheep, you will need to fulfill your dog’s genetically-ingrained
needs by providing some outlet through which he can honor the “herding energy”
within him. If it is not actual herding, he will likely suggest the dog does
agility training. CM works with a lot of German shepherds, highly intelligent
working dogs that need jobs in order to be fulfilled. When they are not
fulfilled, and are therefore what CM calls imbalanced, they will redirect their
energy counterproductively, into activities like digging, barking excessively,
or fixating on shadows. CM says “Breeds were created for different reasons –
some dogs were bred as companions, some as herders, and some as protectors. But
each was bred to draw out and focus on desired instincts to create dogs that
excelled at particular tasks. Although the animal and species aspect of dogs
are common to all of them, breed can sometimes affect behavior, and it is also
sometimes necessary to consider breed when working with a dog, whether just for
training, by giving them an appropriate job, or in rehabilitation (Cesar Millan,
Short Guide to a Happy Dog).
I relate these
ideas of CM’s to the idea of encouraging individuality and freedom of
expression in one’s children. In the parenting literature, support for autonomy
extends beyond allowing children freedom of choice and expression to
communicating genuine respect and deference, and encouraging children to
actively discover, explore, and articulate their own views, goals and
preferences (Skinner, Johnson, Snyder, 2005). Most importantly, labels like
“positive” or “high quality” parenting typically include parenting that is not
only warm but also high in structure and autonomy support. And an optimal
parenting style (e.g. authoritative) is one that combines high structure and
high autonomy support (Skinner, Johnson, Snyder, 2005). All of this being said,
I believe most experts in parenting in concert with experts in canine behavior
(in the context of a wolf pack being akin to a family group, with the mother
and father wolf its leaders) would agree with CM’s formula for balance,
fulfillment and pack leadership.
To hone Cesar’s
methods, one must learn precisely that which cannot be taught in a classroom. Rather,
becoming a calm-assertive pack leader begins with a long, hard look in the mirror.
It involves taking daily inventory of what energy you are projecting, not only
to your dog, but to the world. It is more a spiritual practice with daily
mistakes born of simply “being human”, while always striving to maintain
balance, and get better at your job as guide, teacher, parent and trainer. It
is something that must be brought to life from within a person’s conscious
mind, a form of controlling one’s own behavior in order to see positive effects
in the outward behavior of one’s dog. I, for one, begin again each morning with
the goal of calm-assertive, self-assured, effective and efficient leadership in
mind. And I end each day reflecting on where I’ve grown, and where I still need
work. Ultimately, as human beings, our very best is all we can do. And luckily
for us, our dogs see in us the pack leader we should all see in ourselves.